Measuring Attachment: THE STRANGE SITUATION
Mary Ainsworth and colleagues developed a procedure to measure attachment in one- to two-year-olds and caregivers (1978). This famous experiment, known as the Strange Situation, involves observing the baby’s response to the stresses of a stranger and separation and reunion with the caregiver. The experimental procedure:
This method was created in response to the limitations and criticisms of the Strange Situation methodology (Waters & Deane, 1985). The AQS provides a much more thorough (but time-consuming) assessment. Advantages include testing attachment outside the laboratory setting and using the test with children up to five years old. The test consists of observations for 100 items that cover the span of attachment behaviors. Children are then placed on a continuum that runs from secure to insecure attachments, rather than in one category or another. The ASQ doesn’t provide different classifications in the insecure category.
The way a child responds determines whether his or her attachment pattern is classified as secure or insecure. Each attachment type is characterized by certain behaviors that indicate the quality of the infant-caregiver relationship. Researchers look specifically at two behaviors during the Strange Situation to measure attachment:
Secure attachment:
In the Strange Situation, the securely attached child will explore freely and engage with the stranger because of the security she or he derives from the presence of the parent. The child uses the parent as a secure base and will protest or resist separation from her. When the parent returns, a securely attached child shows joy at the reunion. This is the most adaptive style and is seen in approximately 65% of infants in North America. Babies become securely attached when their primary caregiver responds sensitively to their emotional and physical needs. Care that is consistent, warm, and appropriate, helps the child build positive relationships and learn to regulate stress.
Insecure attachment:
Ainsworth’s research identified two different types of insecure attachment. A third category, disorganized attachment (Main & Solomon, 1990), was later added because of the large number of infants who did not fit into one of the original three classifications. These babies react differently but are the same in their insecurity regarding their primary relationship.
- Avoidant Attachment - These infants ignore or avoid their caregiver and show little reaction to her presence or absence. The avoidant baby treats all adults--from strangers to the attachment figure--in a similar way. This attachment pattern may result from parenting that discourages sadness, pushes independence, and doesn’t offer comfort. This is the pattern of 20% of North American babies.
- Resistant Attachment - This is the attachment type for 10 to 15% of American children. These children do not feel secure enough to explore the room or interact with the stranger. Instead resistant infants will cling to their parent and became very distressed at her absence. But when the parent returns, the child is not comforted and reacts with a mixture of emotions. This explains why this attachment pattern was originally called ambivalent. Inconsistent parenting that is focused on meeting the needs of the parent rather than the infant probably causes resistant attachment. This may be because these parents are often preoccupied with unresolved emotional issues from their own past.
- Disorganized Attachment - This category shows an attachment type so confused that it is an unorganized mix of ambivalent and avoidant patterns. These children will not react to separations and reunions in a coherent fashion. Instead they appear confused or fearful because their caregiver provides a source of stress rather than comfort. Infant behaviors include dazed facial expressions, trance-like withdrawal, and unusual frozen positioning. Disorganized attachment appears to be the result of either frightening or frightened parenting behaviors. Their care can be rejecting, unpredictable, ineffectual, erratic, abusive, or hostile. Parents of children with disorganized attachment usually had childhoods marked with loss and abuse. This style is most maladaptive and is associated with a high risk of later psychopathy (Lyons-Ruth, 1996; Lyons-Ruth, Alpern, & Repacholi, 1993). Seen in 5% of infants in North America.